COYOTES: LISTENING TO TRICKSTERS
As the sunset colors fade from purple
to black, the
forest is dimly illuminated by a first
quarter moon.
An eerie sound breaks the calm. It is
not the long,
low, slow howling of wolves that can
be heard further
north, but the group yip-howl of
coyotes: short howls
that often rise and fall in pitch,
punctuated with
staccato yips, yaps, and barks.
When people hear coyote howls,
they often
mistakenly assume that they’re
hearing a large
pack of animals, all raising their
voices at once.
But this is an auditory illusion
called the “beau
geste” effect. Because of the
variety of sounds
produced by each coyote, and
the way sound is
distorted as it passes through
the environment,
two of these tricksters can sound
like seven or
eight animals.
Group yip-howls are produced
by a mated and
territorial pair of “alpha” coyotes,
with the male
howling while the female
intersperses her yips,
barks, and short howls. “Beta”
coyotes (the
children of the alpha pair from
previous years)
and current year pups may join
in if they are
nearby, or respond with howls
of their own.
And once one group of coyotes
starts howling,
chances are that any other alpha
pairs nearby
will respond in kind, with chorus
after chorus
of group yip-howls rippling across
the miles.
I spent seven years studying coyote
vocal
communication during my dissertation
research
at the University of California, Berkeley.
While
eastern coyotes are a larger and distinct
subspecies from the western coyotes that
I
worked with, the basic findings of my
research
and the work done by others applies to
all coyotes.
Coyotes have sometimes been called
“song dogs,”
and their long distance songs come in
two basic types.
The first, the group yip-howl, is thought
to have
the dual purpose of promoting bonding
within the
family group while also serving as a
territorial
display. In other words, the coyotes
are saying
“we’re a happy family, and we own
this turf so
you better keep out.” In a sense, the
group howls
create an auditory fence around a
territory,
supplementing the physical scent
marks left by
the group.
Coyotes will also howl and bark
separately. This
second type of song is virtually
always an
indication of disturbance or
agitation, and in
my experience, the higher the
proportion of
howls, the more agitated the
coyote is. Coyotes
will howl and bark at neighbors
who intrude
on their territory, and at dogs,
people, and other
large animals that they perceive
as a potential threat.
My research documented that
coyote barks
and howls are individually specific.
Much like
we can tell people apart by their
voices, there
is enough information in coyote
vocalizations
for me (OK, my computer if you
want to get
technical) to tell individuals apart.
If, as I
suspect, coyotes can distinguish
each other
by their song, it would not be
analogous to
the animals constantly shouting
their own
names; it would be more akin to
our ability
to recognize Marlon Brando
because of the
distinctive timbre and cadence
of his voice.
Characteristics including
dominant pitch,
duration, how quickly howls rise
and fall in
pitch, and tendency to “warble”
while howling
all distinguish one coyote from
another.
For howls, this individual
distinctiveness does
not fade with distance. I was
able to record
and identify individual coyotes
over a distance
of greater than one mile. Given
their keen hearing,
it is likely coyotes can discern
individual howls
at much greater distances
—three miles or more
on a calm night.
Barks, on the other hand, degrade
quickly over
distance, with the higher frequencies
fading first.
This makes it theoretically possible
for coyotes
familiar with an individual (say, a
mate or family
group member) to determine
roughly how far
away that individual is, based
on the proportion
of high frequencies in the barks.
Imagine a scenario where a lone
coyote is patrolling
the territory boundary and comes
across an intruder.
He starts barking and howling, and
his mate and
beta children come running to the
right place
because his howls indicate how
agitated he is, and
his barks allow his family to pinpoint
the direction
and distance to his location. Although
I was not able
to prove that coyotes can do these
tasks, the
information needed is present in
their calls and
there are strong evolutionary
advantages to learning
how to use it.
We still have much to learn about
coyote vocal
communication. Even after years
of studying
coyote calls, I was barely able to
scratch the
surface. These tricksters hold their
secrets tightly.
--------------------------------------------------------------------------
Brian Mitchell is an adjunct professor
at the
University of Vermont. Now that he
has kids,
that grad school schedule of getting
up at 2
AM for field work sounds pretty relaxing.
--------------------------------------------------------------------------
they often
mistakenly assume that they’re
hearing a large
pack of animals, all raising their
voices at once.
But this is an auditory illusion
called the “beau
geste” effect. Because of the
variety of sounds
produced by each coyote, and
the way sound is
distorted as it passes through
the environment,
two of these tricksters can sound
like seven or
eight animals.
by a mated and
territorial pair of “alpha” coyotes,
with the male
howling while the female
intersperses her yips,
barks, and short howls. “Beta”
coyotes (the
children of the alpha pair from
previous years)
and current year pups may join
in if they are
nearby, or respond with howls
of their own.
And once one group of coyotes
starts howling,
chances are that any other alpha
pairs nearby
will respond in kind, with chorus
after chorus
of group yip-howls rippling across
the miles.
I spent seven years studying coyote
vocal
communication during my dissertation
research
at the University of California, Berkeley.
While
eastern coyotes are a larger and distinct
subspecies from the western coyotes that
I
worked with, the basic findings of my
research
and the work done by others applies to
all coyotes.
Coyotes have sometimes been called
“song dogs,”
and their long distance songs come in
two basic types.
to have
the dual purpose of promoting bonding
within the
family group while also serving as a
territorial
display. In other words, the coyotes
are saying
“we’re a happy family, and we own
this turf so
you better keep out.” In a sense, the
group howls
create an auditory fence around a
territory,
supplementing the physical scent
marks left by
the group.
separately. This
second type of song is virtually
always an
indication of disturbance or
agitation, and in
my experience, the higher the
proportion of
howls, the more agitated the
coyote is. Coyotes
will howl and bark at neighbors
who intrude
on their territory, and at dogs,
people, and other
large animals that they perceive
as a potential threat.
coyote barks
and howls are individually specific.
Much like
we can tell people apart by their
voices, there
is enough information in coyote
vocalizations
for me (OK, my computer if you
want to get
technical) to tell individuals apart.
If, as I
suspect, coyotes can distinguish
each other
by their song, it would not be
analogous to
the animals constantly shouting
their own
names; it would be more akin to
our ability
to recognize Marlon Brando
because of the
distinctive timbre and cadence
of his voice.
Characteristics including
dominant pitch,
duration, how quickly howls rise
and fall in
pitch, and tendency to “warble”
while howling
all distinguish one coyote from
another.
distinctiveness does
not fade with distance. I was
able to record
and identify individual coyotes
over a distance
of greater than one mile. Given
their keen hearing,
it is likely coyotes can discern
individual howls
at much greater distances
—three miles or more
on a calm night.
quickly over
distance, with the higher frequencies
fading first.
This makes it theoretically possible
for coyotes
familiar with an individual (say, a
mate or family
group member) to determine
roughly how far
away that individual is, based
on the proportion
of high frequencies in the barks.
coyote is patrolling
the territory boundary and comes
across an intruder.
He starts barking and howling, and
his mate and
beta children come running to the
right place
because his howls indicate how
agitated he is, and
his barks allow his family to pinpoint
the direction
and distance to his location. Although
I was not able
to prove that coyotes can do these
tasks, the
information needed is present in
their calls and
there are strong evolutionary
advantages to learning
how to use it.
coyote vocal
communication. Even after years
of studying
coyote calls, I was barely able to
scratch the
surface. These tricksters hold their
secrets tightly.
--------------------------------------------------------------------------
Brian Mitchell is an adjunct professor
at the
University of Vermont. Now that he
has kids,
that grad school schedule of getting
up at 2
AM for field work sounds pretty relaxing.
--------------------------------------------------------------------------
at the
University of Vermont. Now that he
has kids,
that grad school schedule of getting
up at 2
AM for field work sounds pretty relaxing.
--------------------------------------------------------------------------
No comments:
Post a Comment